Carr, Bierman find canal between ears helps alligators pinpoint sound

news story image

Alligator that participated in a University of Maryland study that found that the alligator's ear is strongly directional because of large, air-filled channels connecting the two middle ears. The earflap opening behind the alligator's eye can be seen clearly in this photo.

 
Photographer: Hilary Bierman

By reptile standards, alligators are positively chatty. They are the most vocal of the non-avian reptiles and are known to be able to pinpoint the source of sounds with accuracy. But it wasn’t clear exactly how they did it because they lack external auditory structures.

A University of Maryland study found that alligators' ears are strongly directional because of large, air-filled channels connecting the two middle ears. The earflap opening behind the alligator's eye can be seen in this photo. 

In a new study, an international team of biologists shows that the alligator’s ear is strongly directional because of large, air-filled channels connecting the two middle ears. This configuration is similar in birds, which have an interaural canal that increases directionality.

“Mammals usually have large moveable ears, but alligators do not, so they have solved the problems of sound localization a little differently. This may also be the solution used by the alligator's dinosaur relatives,” said Hilary Bierman, a biology lecturer at the University of Maryland.

The study, which was led by Bierman and UMD Biology Professor Catherine Carr, was published online in the Journal of Experimental Biology on March 26, 2014. The research was funded by the National Institutes of Health, the National Science Foundation, the Danish National Science Foundation and the Carlsberg Foundation.

The UMD biologists—along with researchers from the University of Massachusetts Lowell, University of Colorado Medical School and University of Southern Denmark—collected anatomical, biophysical and electrophysiological measurements of alligators to investigate the mechanisms alligators use to locate sounds.

“Different vertebrate lineages have evolved external and/or internal anatomical adaptations to enhance these auditory cues, such as pinnae and interaural canals,” said Bierman.

First, the team tested how sound traveled around an alligator’s head to investigate whether the animal somehow channels sound, listening for tiny time and volume differences in the sound’s arrival at the two ears to help locate the origin. But the team found no evidence that the animal’s body alters sound transmission sufficiently for the animal to be able to detect the difference. And when the team measured alligators’ brainstem responses to sounds, they were too fast for the animals to sense these small time differences.

Next, the team looked for internal structures in the alligators’ heads that might propagate sound between the two eardrums. Viewing slices through the heads of young alligators, the team could clearly see two channels linking the two middle ears that could transmit sound between the two eardrums.

Sound reaches both sides of the eardrum—traveling externally to reach the outer side and through head structures to the internal side—to amplify the vibration at some frequencies when the head is aligned with the sound. This maximizes the pressure differences on the two sides of the eardrum, magnifying the time difference between the sound arriving at the ear drum via two different paths to allow the animal to pinpoint the source. And when the team looked at the eardrum’s vibration, they could see that it was amplified at certain frequencies, as they would expect if alligators use the pressure difference at the eardrum for orientation.

Assembling all of the evidence together, the researchers suggest that the reptiles rely on magnified time difference at the eardrum to locate noises. They also suspect that this is the mechanism that the archosaur ancestors of modern crocodilians and birds used to pinpoint sounds.

This research was supported by the National Institute on Deafness and Other Communication Disorders of the National Institutes of Health under Award Nos. DC-00436, DC-04664 and DC-011555; the National Science Foundation under Award No. 0817208; the Danish National Science Foundation under Award No. DFF1323-00132; and the Carlsberg Foundation. The content of this article does not necessarily reflect the views of the funding organizations.

The research paper, “Biophysics of directional hearing in the American alligator (Alligator mississippiensis),” Hilary S. Bierman, Jennifer L. Thornton, Heath G. Jones, Kanthaiah Koka, Bruce A. Young, Christian Brandt, Jakob Christensen-Dalsgaard, Catherine E. Carr and Daniel J. Tollin, published online on March 26, 2014 in the Journal of Experimental Biology.

Story by: Kathryn Knight (JEB) and Abby Robinson (UMD)

Published March 26, 2014